You Were Probably Taught to Read Wrong | Otherwords

00:08:45
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=bGsNcFfezLM

Sintesi

TLDRThe video discusses the decades-long debate over the best method to teach reading to children, focusing on phonics versus whole language theory. Phonics emphasizes teaching children to read by correlating sounds with letters, while whole language theory promotes a natural acquisition of reading skills through exposure to books. The video highlights the shortcomings of whole language theory, particularly the use of the three cueing system, which encourages guessing words from context rather than sounding them out. Cognitive research using eye-tracking and MRI technology has supported phonics, showing that skilled readers process all letters and associate them with sounds. The failure of whole language methods has led some educators to return to phonics, especially after renewed parental scrutiny during COVID-19, when many realized their children's reading skills were lacking. The video suggests that understanding the history of reading instruction can provide insights into cognitive processes and emphasize the importance of phonics in literacy development.

Punti di forza

  • 🧩 Phonics and whole language theory are at the center of a major education debate.
  • 🔍 Whole language theory relies on context clues rather than sounding out words.
  • 🧠 Cognitive science shows skilled readers observe every letter and connect them to sounds.
  • 📉 Whole language theory linked to a proficiency crisis in early literacy.
  • 🎓 Many educators and parents now advocate for a return to phonics-based instruction.
  • 🔄 COVID-19 highlighted issues in reading instruction methods for parents.
  • 👁️ Eye-tracking technology challenged the assumptions of whole language theory.
  • 📚 Literacy rates rose dramatically in the 20th century, complicating perceptions of reading.
  • 🚸 The majority of children require structured phonics instruction in literacy development.
  • 📡 Advancement in science has reshaped the methods of teaching reading.

Linea temporale

  • 00:00:00 - 00:08:45

    The video discusses the controversial theory of reading, which involves jumbled letters in words being readable if the first and last letters remain the same. This theory implies reading might be a loose process, where our brains use context to deduce words. The debate around teaching children to read, between phonics or whole language theory, is highlighted. Whole language theory, proposed by Ken Goodman, suggests reading is natural like speaking. But recent discoveries suggest phonics, which correlates letters to sounds, may be essential for proficient reading.

Mappa mentale

Mind Map

Domande frequenti

  • What is the main debate discussed in the video?

    The video discusses the ongoing debate on the best method to teach children to read, focusing on phonics versus whole language theory.

  • How does phonics teach children to read?

    Phonics teaches reading by correlating alphabetic symbols to vocal sounds, helping children sound out words.

  • What is whole language theory?

    Whole language theory suggests that reading is a natural, intuitive process where children can learn to read on their own with enough exposure to books, similar to how they learn to walk and talk.

  • What is the three cueing system?

    The three cueing system is a strategy where children use context clues to guess unfamiliar words instead of sounding them out, utilizing visual, syntactic, and semantic cues.

  • Why has whole language theory faced criticism?

    Whole language theory has been criticized for leading to increased literacy issues, as cognitive science shows skilled readers sound out words and do not rely on guessing from context.

  • What role did eye-tracking and MRI technology play in this debate?

    Eye-tracking and MRI studies have shown that skilled readers observe every letter and connect words with their sounds, challenging the whole language theory's assumptions.

  • How did COVID-19 influence perceptions on reading methods?

    During COVID-19, parents observed their children's reading lessons over Zoom and noticed issues with the cueing method, prompting a shift back towards phonics instruction.

  • What are some arguments against the cueing method?

    Critics argue that cueing cultivates bad reading habits leading to higher error rates and poorer reading proficiency compared to phonics.

Visualizza altre sintesi video

Ottenete l'accesso immediato ai riassunti gratuiti dei video di YouTube grazie all'intelligenza artificiale!
Sottotitoli
en
Scorrimento automatico:
  • 00:00:00
    - Can you read this sentence?
  • 00:00:02
    You've probably seen something like this
  • 00:00:03
    on the internet before.
  • 00:00:05
    The letters of the words are all jumbled up,
  • 00:00:07
    but supposedly, as long as the first
  • 00:00:10
    and sometimes last letters of each word stay the same,
  • 00:00:13
    somehow we can read it.
  • 00:00:15
    This quirky language game seems to suggest
  • 00:00:18
    that reading is a pretty inexact process.
  • 00:00:21
    Our eyes skim the print,
  • 00:00:22
    we notice the length of the word and maybe the first letter;
  • 00:00:26
    then our brains use context clues
  • 00:00:28
    to deduce what the word should be.
  • 00:00:30
    But is that really how reading works?
  • 00:00:32
    Believe it or not, this question
  • 00:00:34
    is at the heart of a decades-long battle
  • 00:00:37
    in the English-speaking world,
  • 00:00:38
    a battle that's been raging
  • 00:00:40
    amongst educators, politicians, and scientists
  • 00:00:43
    about what is the best way to teach children to read?
  • 00:00:46
    Even as I speak, school districts across the US
  • 00:00:49
    are facing the possibility that the whole theory
  • 00:00:52
    they've been using to teach kids to read is totally wrong.
  • 00:00:56
    Whether or not this means the wars are truly over,
  • 00:00:59
    digging into the history can teach us a lot
  • 00:01:01
    about how our brains work,
  • 00:01:03
    and offer a new appreciation
  • 00:01:04
    for one of mankind's greatest inventions: the written word.
  • 00:01:09
    I'm Dr. Erica Brozovsky, and this is "Otherwords."
  • 00:01:12
    (bright whimsical music)
  • 00:01:19
    - [Announcer] "Otherwords."
  • 00:01:22
    (crowd cheering)
  • 00:01:23
    - The '60s and '70s were times
  • 00:01:25
    of great social upheaval in the West;
  • 00:01:27
    longstanding conventions were being challenged,
  • 00:01:30
    and society was being reimagined
  • 00:01:32
    to give individuals more freedom and autonomy.
  • 00:01:35
    Against this backdrop,
  • 00:01:36
    a novel theory of how children learn to read
  • 00:01:38
    was introduced by language professor Ken Goodman.
  • 00:01:42
    Up until this time,
  • 00:01:43
    most children learned to read through phonics,
  • 00:01:45
    which taught the correlation
  • 00:01:47
    of alphabetic symbols to vocal sounds.
  • 00:01:49
    But Goodman's whole language theory
  • 00:01:52
    asserted that children didn't learn to read
  • 00:01:54
    by sounding out words;
  • 00:01:56
    instead, the process was much more natural and intuitive.
  • 00:02:00
    Give a kid enough freedom and access to books,
  • 00:02:02
    and they'll discover how to read on their own,
  • 00:02:04
    much like babies learn to walk and talk.
  • 00:02:06
    New Zealand literacy researcher Marie Clay
  • 00:02:09
    developed a teaching program
  • 00:02:10
    based on whole language theory,
  • 00:02:12
    which incorporated a strategy
  • 00:02:14
    known as cueing or three cueing.
  • 00:02:16
    The idea is that when confronted with an unfamiliar word,
  • 00:02:20
    the natural solution is not to sound it out
  • 00:02:22
    but to guess it using context clues.
  • 00:02:25
    These clues were grouped into three categories,
  • 00:02:28
    hence three cueing:
  • 00:02:29
    visual, which meant the shape of the word or its spelling;
  • 00:02:32
    syntactic, which is the sentence structure and grammar;
  • 00:02:35
    and semantic, the meaning of the passage,
  • 00:02:38
    which would even include illustrations.
  • 00:02:40
    For instance, if the child doesn't know this word,
  • 00:02:43
    they could use the sentence structure
  • 00:02:44
    to guess that it's a noun,
  • 00:02:46
    the illustration to guess its meaning,
  • 00:02:48
    and the first letter to triangulate to a solution
  • 00:02:51
    that fits all three categories.
  • 00:02:53
    Clay's program was adopted
  • 00:02:54
    by all New Zealand schools by 1983,
  • 00:02:57
    and then imported to America,
  • 00:02:59
    where versions of it were popularized
  • 00:03:01
    by education professors like Lucy Calkins,
  • 00:03:03
    Irene Fountas, and Gay Su Pinnell.
  • 00:03:05
    An important component of the method, proponents claimed,
  • 00:03:09
    was that reading always be perceived
  • 00:03:10
    as a pleasurable, self-guided activity.
  • 00:03:13
    Lessons were brief, and students were encouraged
  • 00:03:16
    to spend lots of time reading alone.
  • 00:03:18
    Phonics should be limited or avoided altogether,
  • 00:03:21
    because it could make reading feel tedious and dull.
  • 00:03:24
    If you instill a love and enthusiasm for reading early on,
  • 00:03:27
    the thinking went,
  • 00:03:28
    kids will become skilled readers on their own.
  • 00:03:31
    It was a philosophy that resonated
  • 00:03:33
    with a lot of people at the time.
  • 00:03:35
    And by the 1990s,
  • 00:03:36
    cueing had come to dominate reading curricula
  • 00:03:39
    in school districts across the country.
  • 00:03:41
    And yet since its implementation,
  • 00:03:43
    America has experienced a crisis in early literacy,
  • 00:03:47
    with less than a third of fourth graders
  • 00:03:49
    being able to read at a proficient level.
  • 00:03:51
    Why?
  • 00:03:53
    Cognitive scientists were making new discoveries
  • 00:03:55
    that were casting serious doubts
  • 00:03:57
    on the foundations of whole language theory.
  • 00:03:59
    Eye-tracking devices showed that skilled readers,
  • 00:04:02
    even when reading quickly,
  • 00:04:04
    are still observing every letter in a word,
  • 00:04:06
    not just the first one.
  • 00:04:08
    MRIs revealed that even when a word
  • 00:04:10
    has been orthographically mapped,
  • 00:04:12
    it still lights up the speech and listening centers
  • 00:04:14
    of our brains.
  • 00:04:15
    This means that even when we know a word so well
  • 00:04:18
    as to recognize its meaning instantly,
  • 00:04:20
    our brains are still on some level
  • 00:04:22
    connecting it with its sound.
  • 00:04:24
    Similarly, studies were showing that skilled readers
  • 00:04:27
    were more likely to sound out unfamiliar words.
  • 00:04:30
    Guessing words through context
  • 00:04:31
    was a strategy more often used by poor readers,
  • 00:04:34
    and it led to more mistakes than sounding them out.
  • 00:04:37
    Proponents of the science of reading,
  • 00:04:39
    as it came to be called,
  • 00:04:40
    claimed that Marie Clay and her disciples
  • 00:04:42
    had it exactly backwards.
  • 00:04:44
    Instead of decoding words to reveal the meaning of the text,
  • 00:04:48
    they were asking students to use the meaning of a text
  • 00:04:50
    to decode the words.
  • 00:04:52
    Let's revisit that sentence at the beginning of the video.
  • 00:04:55
    You can technically guess the words
  • 00:04:57
    using context, syntax, and initial letters,
  • 00:05:00
    but I can guarantee reading this sentence
  • 00:05:02
    would be much, much faster.
  • 00:05:04
    Furthermore, if we start to increase the complexity
  • 00:05:07
    and obscurity of the words,
  • 00:05:09
    your accuracy rates will start to plummet.
  • 00:05:11
    Could you read a whole book like this?
  • 00:05:14
    Maybe, but it wouldn't be fun.
  • 00:05:16
    It would take forever.
  • 00:05:18
    You'd make a lot of mistakes,
  • 00:05:19
    and your mind would be so taxed
  • 00:05:21
    from trying to decipher the words
  • 00:05:23
    you wouldn't have much mental energy left
  • 00:05:25
    to ponder the content.
  • 00:05:26
    As clear as the science seemed to be,
  • 00:05:29
    cueing was too firmly established by this time
  • 00:05:31
    to be dislodged easily.
  • 00:05:33
    Most teachers had been thoroughly trained in the approach,
  • 00:05:36
    and didn't like scientists telling them
  • 00:05:37
    how to run their classrooms.
  • 00:05:39
    The gurus of the movement, Calkins, Fountas, and Pinnell
  • 00:05:42
    had become celebrities of the educational world,
  • 00:05:45
    making millions of dollars selling their curricula
  • 00:05:48
    to schools across the country.
  • 00:05:49
    The only concession they made to the science
  • 00:05:52
    was to include a bit more phonics in their lessons
  • 00:05:54
    and repackage it as balanced literacy.
  • 00:05:58
    But scientists countered that any amount of cueing
  • 00:06:01
    cultivated bad reading habits
  • 00:06:03
    that could last into adulthood.
  • 00:06:04
    According to education journalist Emily Hanford,
  • 00:06:07
    it wasn't until COVID hit
  • 00:06:08
    that things really began to change.
  • 00:06:11
    Suddenly, millions of parents
  • 00:06:12
    were sitting right next to their kids
  • 00:06:14
    during their reading classes over Zoom,
  • 00:06:16
    and a lot of them didn't like what they were hearing.
  • 00:06:19
    Despite being told that their child
  • 00:06:20
    was reading at the appropriate level,
  • 00:06:22
    it was obvious that some had just memorized the lesson.
  • 00:06:25
    And when confronted with a truly unfamiliar word,
  • 00:06:28
    they lacked the skills to decode it.
  • 00:06:30
    Hanford's six-part podcast, "Sold a Story,"
  • 00:06:34
    which chronicles the failures of cueing
  • 00:06:36
    as a teaching method,
  • 00:06:37
    spread quickly amongst outraged parents and educators.
  • 00:06:40
    Around the same time, school districts
  • 00:06:42
    that reinstated systematic explicit phonics instruction
  • 00:06:46
    saw dramatic rises in proficiency scores.
  • 00:06:49
    Although cueing is still used in many classrooms,
  • 00:06:51
    more and more cities and states are reverting back
  • 00:06:54
    to what was essentially the system
  • 00:06:55
    for teaching reading prior to 1960,
  • 00:06:58
    and getting promising results.
  • 00:07:00
    Even Lucy Calkins admitted that her curriculum
  • 00:07:02
    had failed to keep pace with the scientific evidence.
  • 00:07:05
    Does this mean that the era of cueing is finally ending?
  • 00:07:09
    Has science won the reading wars?
  • 00:07:11
    Only time will tell.
  • 00:07:13
    But it does seem clear that the theory of reading
  • 00:07:16
    that originated half a century ago
  • 00:07:17
    with Ken Goodman and Marie Clay had two fundamental flaws.
  • 00:07:22
    First, it assumed that acquisition of literacy
  • 00:07:24
    works the same way as acquisition of speech,
  • 00:07:27
    but our brains and vocal chords evolved for speech:
  • 00:07:31
    a baby is hardwired to learn how to talk
  • 00:07:33
    without any formal instruction;
  • 00:07:35
    writing was only invented about 5,000 years ago.
  • 00:07:38
    Sure, there are a small number of kids
  • 00:07:40
    who can unravel the code on their own,
  • 00:07:43
    but the vast majority of us need to be taught.
  • 00:07:45
    Second, it minimized the importance of sound in literacy.
  • 00:07:49
    Connecting those squiggles on the page to phonemes
  • 00:07:52
    allows us to leverage
  • 00:07:53
    the powerful speech and listening centers of our brains.
  • 00:07:57
    It makes reading feel as natural and effortless
  • 00:07:59
    as listening to someone talk.
  • 00:08:01
    It's weird that it took all these advancements
  • 00:08:04
    in science and technology to bring us back to the method
  • 00:08:08
    that had been the standard for hundreds of years.
  • 00:08:10
    But perhaps reading was a victim of its own success.
  • 00:08:14
    Literacy rates skyrocketed in the 20th century;
  • 00:08:17
    from newspapers to advertisements to the internet,
  • 00:08:20
    the written word has become such a ubiquitous,
  • 00:08:23
    ever-present part of our environment.
  • 00:08:25
    It's easy to forget that it's actually a human invention;
  • 00:08:29
    one we've become so good at you'd think it came naturally.
  • 00:08:33
    The gurus of the moment, movement, movement.
  • 00:08:36
    The gurus of the moment, movement...
  • 00:08:40
    Phonics is dull. Yeah.
Tag
  • reading
  • phonics
  • whole language
  • literacy
  • education
  • teaching methods
  • cueing system
  • cognitive science
  • reading wars
  • literacy crisis