Color (Ch 10) I, Visualization Analysis & Design, 2021

00:18:55
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=QNDd_hvdORw

Résumé

TLDRIn this discussion, the focus is on the effective use of color in visualization. The video emphasizes decomposing color into three separate channels: luminance (brightness), saturation (colorful intensity), and hue (the actual color, such as red or blue). These channels can be used to effectively encode data based on whether it is categorical or ordered. Key points include the limitation of using a small number of discriminable color bins (6 to 12) and avoiding rainbow color maps for ordered data due to their non-linear perceptual interpretation. The video also stresses the complexity of using bivariate color maps and encourages utilizing other visual channels alongside color. When designing color palettes, one must consider whether the data is segmented or continuous, and choose appropriate sequential, diverging, or cyclic properties. Moreover, tools like Colorbrewer can aid in creating effective color palettes based on these principles. Lastly, the intricacies of using transparency as a channel, often confused with luminance and saturation, are discussed, advocating for careful consideration when employing color in visual data representation.

A retenir

  • 🎨 Decompose color into luminance, saturation, and hue for effective visualization.
  • 🔍 Human perception prefers relative color comparisons, especially when colors are contiguous.
  • 📊 Limit categorically colored bins to 6-12 for effective differentiation.
  • 🌈 Avoid rainbow color maps for ordered data due to perceptual confusion.
  • 🌀 Use cyclic color maps to emphasize the cyclical nature of data.
  • 🚫 Be cautious using transparency as it intertwines with luminance and saturation.
  • 📐 Consider using other visual channels along with color for better data encoding.
  • 🎯 For small regions, use highly saturated colors to maintain visibility.
  • 🔧 Tools like Colorbrewer assist in generating well-designed color palettes.
  • 🖼️ Choose between sequential, diverging, or cyclic palettes based on data properties.

Chronologie

  • 00:00:00 - 00:05:00

    The segment begins with an exploration of color as a visual encoding channel, expanding beyond spatial arrangements. Color is broken down into three channels: luminance (brightness), saturation (colorfulness), and hue (identity channel). These channels serve different purposes, such as ordered and categorical attributes. Focusing on color channels, the importance of understanding and utilizing them correctly is highlighted, especially in visualization contexts where the number of discriminable bins is a consideration. Examples illustrate how misuse of color can lead to confusion, particularly with non-contiguous, small regions.

  • 00:05:00 - 00:10:00

    The discussion shifts to the proper use of categorical and ordered colors, emphasizing the limited discriminable bins usually available (6-12 bins). It advises on using additional visual channels and deliberate binning for categorical colors. The segment also critiques the common misuse of rainbow color ordering due to perceived ordering inconsistencies, explaining how luminance-ordered sequential colors like Viridis and magma are preferable for sequential data and how matching visual encoding to dataset characteristics is critical. It touches on issues like the intrinsic perceptual ordering of colors and the impact of size on color salience.

  • 00:10:00 - 00:18:55

    The final segment delves into color palette design and its considerations, such as categorical, sequential, and diverging color maps. It explains how sequential maps go from min to max values, while diverging maps emphasize a midpoint, often using neutral colors. It also covers the complexity of designing bivariate color maps for two attributes, especially when both possess multiple levels. The segment wraps with a focus on ensuring color palettes are perceptually linear, appropriately ordered by luminance, and colorblind safe, providing examples and caveats in the use of such color mappings.

Carte mentale

Mind Map

Questions fréquemment posées

  • What are the three channels of color in visualization?

    The three channels are luminance, saturation, and hue.

  • How many discriminable bins are recommended for categorical color?

    Only between 6 and 12 bins are recommended.

  • Why should one avoid using rainbow color maps for ordered data?

    Rainbows lack a perceptual intrinsic ordering and can cause nonlinear visual interpretation.

  • What is a useful rule of thumb for using categorical color in visualization?

    Use only between 6 and 12 bins, considering the need for background and highlight colors.

  • How are sequential and diverging color maps different?

    Sequential maps go from min to max, while diverging maps emphasize a semantically meaningful midpoint.

  • How does size affect color perception in visualization?

    Smaller regions require highly saturated colors to be noticeable.

  • Why is transparency difficult to use for encoding data?

    Transparency is not separable from luminance and saturation.

  • What should be remembered when using bivariate color maps?

    Bivariate color maps with multiple levels in each direction are difficult to interpret.

  • What are some key considerations when designing color palettes?

    Consider if the data is segmented or continuous, and choose sequential, diverging, or cyclic properties.

  • What tool is recommended for generating color palettes?

    Colorbrewer is recommended for generating informed color palettes.

Voir plus de résumés vidéo

Accédez instantanément à des résumés vidéo gratuits sur YouTube grâce à l'IA !
Sous-titres
en
Défilement automatique:
  • 00:00:01
    Let's continue talking about Visualization  Analysis and Design by diving into color.
  • 00:00:07
    So, when we think about ways of visually encoding  so far, we've been pretty focused on spatial
  • 00:00:15
    arrangements of attributes. Now let's switch  over and go beyond just spatial arrangement,
  • 00:00:21
    and think about mapping to some of the  other visual channels, particularly color.
  • 00:00:28
    So what is going on with color? When I was  talking about marks and channels before,
  • 00:00:32
    there were these three different things that  had color in them. Let's think about that.
  • 00:00:38
    So, we need to decompose color, because the  first rule of color is: don't talk about color.
  • 00:00:44
    Specifically don't just talk about  color. It's extremely confusing
  • 00:00:48
    if we treat that as monolithic. What we  really need to do is decompose this into
  • 00:00:54
    three channels that have  different characteristics.
  • 00:00:57
    So two of these are magnitude channels  that are good for ordered attributes. The
  • 00:01:03
    luminance channel is how bright something is  - think of that as grayscale black and white.
  • 00:01:08
    And then the saturation channel  is how colorful something is. So,
  • 00:01:13
    how much pink is there in between gray  and light pink and fully saturated pink.
  • 00:01:19
    And then the third channel is an identity  channel. And that's good for showing ordered
  • 00:01:24
    attributes. So color, specifically hue,  is what we usually think of as color.
  • 00:01:30
    And so we can ask questions like what color  is that? Is it red? Is it blue? Is it green?
  • 00:01:35
    So, we've got that identity versus magnitude,  and that is what helps us think about color.
  • 00:01:42
    These channels have different properties. So what  do they convey directly to the perceptual system?
  • 00:01:47
    How much information can they  convey in terms of how many
  • 00:01:51
    discriminable bins can we use?  And so we'll dive into that.
  • 00:01:56
    So, let's talk about color  channels in visualization.
  • 00:02:01
    So, the question of whether we  think of color as categorical
  • 00:02:05
    or ordered is going to depend.  We can have it either way. Right,
  • 00:02:09
    in our upper left corner we're really focusing on  the fact that there are four different years here,
  • 00:02:14
    and those different changes of hue for 2010 versus  2013 versus 2011 distinguishes them as categories.
  • 00:02:22
    And in contrast, in the lower left, we're  much more emphasizing an order of these years,
  • 00:02:28
    as we go from light green, to darker green, to the  darkest green there is from 2010 through to 2013.
  • 00:02:34
    And we're doing something similar on the  upper right, where we've got a choropleth
  • 00:02:39
    map, where we're using color saturation  within geographic regions to color code,
  • 00:02:44
    and then we're seeing the relationship  between that and this bar chart.
  • 00:02:48
    And we obviously have redundantly encoded
  • 00:02:51
    the length of the bar, and the amount  of green in that bar, the saturation.
  • 00:02:57
    So we can see examples that color  really can be either one of these.
  • 00:03:04
    Now when we're thinking about categorical color,  the thing that a lot of people unfortunately get
  • 00:03:09
    wrong is, they want to encode more  levels of a categorical attribute
  • 00:03:14
    than there are available discriminable  bins for categorical color.
  • 00:03:20
    So remember that we talked a lot when  we were introducing marks and channels,
  • 00:03:23
    about the idea that human perception is  completely built on relative comparisons.
  • 00:03:29
    So that's fantastic if there's colored  blocks that are next to each other.
  • 00:03:33
    If they're contiguous, we have very  high precision ability to discriminate.
  • 00:03:38
    Here we've got a 21 chromosomes  on a mouse and we're color coding.
  • 00:03:43
    And even really subtle distinctions between  greens, like in chromosomes six through
  • 00:03:48
    nine, we're able to tell the difference  when they're right next to each other.
  • 00:03:52
    But, and here's something that's gone  wrong, when they now try to map those
  • 00:03:59
    into small and scattered regions - right  here what they've done is they've said okay
  • 00:04:06
    for a mouse chromosome where does it fall  in the human chromosome - let's see how
  • 00:04:10
    we're doing when we have these absolute  comparisons of small scattered regions.
  • 00:04:14
    Well, we've got tan, and green, brown, white, red,  blue, now we're at the top of chromosome 2. Okay,
  • 00:04:22
    light blue, purple, wait is that yellow,  is that the same tan we saw before ...
  • 00:04:28
    see a green ,is that the same green  as before, maybe there's another one.
  • 00:04:34
    So, I'm starting to, I really haven't  even run out of fingers but I'm starting
  • 00:04:38
    to lose the ability to discriminate these subtle  differences in color because they're separated.
  • 00:04:45
    And so it turns out that if you've got  non-contiguous small regions of color,
  • 00:04:50
    it's almost always fewer bins than people  wish that they had available to them.
  • 00:04:54
    So the rule of thumb is only between  6 and 12 bins can you really count on.
  • 00:04:59
    Remembering that you need a color for the  background, maybe you need a default color
  • 00:05:03
    for something, you might need a highlight color. So in general, it's a lot fewer bins than people
  • 00:05:10
    actually would like to use. So what can you do?
  • 00:05:14
    Well one thing you could remember is, you don't  only have color. You have other visual channels.
  • 00:05:20
    If you do want to encode with categorical  color, deliberately yourself, bin things
  • 00:05:27
    and then map those into color. Right now what's happening is, chromosomes
  • 00:05:31
    say 5 through 11, are all getting roughly mapped  into the same bin by our eyes. It might be better
  • 00:05:38
    if we could do a more semantic meaningful binning  rather than just having the luck of the draw in
  • 00:05:43
    terms of which things get binned involuntarily  by us not being able to notice the difference.
  • 00:05:52
    Just soIi don't only pick on the  genomics folks, here's another example
  • 00:05:57
    of people using more discriminable bins than we  can really see. Notice how that light blue for
  • 00:06:03
    cancer is pretty hard to tell apart from  the light blue for ear, nose, and throat.
  • 00:06:09
    So we really do have a limit as to the number  of categories. Think carefully yourself
  • 00:06:15
    about re: transforming your data, deriving a new
  • 00:06:20
    categorical attribute with fewer bins if  you insist on using categorical color.
  • 00:06:26
    This is also an issue for ordered color. How many  bins can we discriminate? Often not quite as many
  • 00:06:32
    as people might like. This is a nice example from  Gregor Aisch, showing how if we use the same basic
  • 00:06:38
    idea of a choropleth map, of color coding onto  geographic regions, well, what do we notice?
  • 00:06:44
    When we just look at those legends across  the top it's always quite clear what the
  • 00:06:48
    differences are between neighboring bins. But then, when we look at the scattering of
  • 00:06:52
    U.S. states - well great for two classes; we can  definitely tell those apart. For three classes,
  • 00:06:58
    no problem at all. For four  classes, still pretty good.
  • 00:07:02
    Now once we even start getting to five classes,  it might be a little bit tricky to distinguish.
  • 00:07:08
    Is Maine the same color as Utah?Probably?  But now once we start getting to six, seven,
  • 00:07:15
    and eight classes, it gets very difficult to tell  apart when things are separate from each other.
  • 00:07:22
    Understanding if West Virginia and Arizona  are the same color with the eight class one;
  • 00:07:26
    it's very difficult. So, remember that we have a
  • 00:07:30
    limited number of discriminable bins if things are  separated as opposed to right next to each other.
  • 00:07:38
    The other thing we have to  remember with ordered data is,
  • 00:07:41
    everyone wants to use rainbows,  but they're really a poor default.
  • 00:07:45
    Now why is that? People get a little confused, I  think, about the physics of reality, which is yes,
  • 00:07:51
    sunlight going through a prism will scatter  into a rainbow. That's a physical fact.
  • 00:07:56
    But the biology of our eyes is  not actually necessarily using
  • 00:08:01
    that information in the way people hope.  It's not intrinsic to have the ordering
  • 00:08:06
    of the rainbow in terms of our perception. If I lock two people into separate rooms with
  • 00:08:10
    no way to communicate, and I give them four  color chips of purple, and red, and orange,
  • 00:08:16
    and blue, are they going to put them in the  same order if I ask them to do that? Probably
  • 00:08:21
    not. I sure wouldn't bet a thousand dollars. But if I had people in rooms and I said, "Okay,
  • 00:08:27
    what's the order in terms of if I got like a light  gray, and a white, and a black, and a dark gray."
  • 00:08:32
    Would they put them in the same order? Surely yes. So remember we want this perceptual intrinsic
  • 00:08:38
    ordering, as opposed to anything that you  have to learn. People can learn, but we want
  • 00:08:43
    with the perceptual stuff to make sure that it's  something intrinsic to the human visual system.
  • 00:08:48
    But that's not the only problem - is the lack of  intrinsic ordering - there's also a non-linearity.
  • 00:08:54
    I'm going to say here's two regions of that  spectrum and notice how within the one on
  • 00:09:00
    the left I can go from red to orange to yellow. I  can clearly see at least three different colors.
  • 00:09:06
    And in a region of the same size i  see green, green, green, on the right.
  • 00:09:10
    Because of the way that the human eye responds to  the visual spectrum it is not a linear situation.
  • 00:09:19
    But, what are some of the benefits of  rainbows? It's not that they're all bad;
  • 00:09:23
    it's that we can actually have fine-grained  structure be visible and enable. And in
  • 00:09:28
    this picture we can talk about the red parts  versus the yellow parts versus the green parts.
  • 00:09:32
    That makes us focus on the fine grain. Now in contrast with the one on the bottom,
  • 00:09:39
    it can actually be difficult to tell what's going  on. We'll come back to that one in a minute.
  • 00:09:45
    So let's compare a different color  map for that same data set on the top.
  • 00:09:51
    And when we just have two hues  going from purple through gray
  • 00:09:55
    into yellow we're seeing a really  different sense of this dataset.
  • 00:10:00
    We are much more able to focus on large  scale structure. So, it's harder to focus
  • 00:10:05
    on fine grain but it's easier to focus on large  scale. That is then a choice of the designer.
  • 00:10:12
    Now here we see an example  where the mystery is cleared up;
  • 00:10:15
    it's actually the coastline of Florida. And there's something else going on here
  • 00:10:19
    there's a very carefully chosen change of  hue at that zero point, to really distinguish
  • 00:10:25
    the blues getting darker as we go down into the  depths, and then the heights of the mountains.
  • 00:10:31
    Something careful was done here. The luminance is  increasing, so we do have multiple colors - but
  • 00:10:38
    they're ordered by the luminance. So  we're going from dark up to bright.
  • 00:10:43
    So that's actually a general principle of  color map design. There's some nice ones.
  • 00:10:48
    Viridis and magma are great for sequential color,  carefully designed and now deployed in many tools.
  • 00:10:54
    And in these, the luminance is monotonically  increasing. It's the hues are ordered by
  • 00:11:01
    luminance. They have other nice properties.  They're perceptually uniform. They do have
  • 00:11:05
    multiple colors in them. And as we'll  talk about later they are colorblind safe.
  • 00:11:12
    So, these are both some useful  color maps for sequential data.
  • 00:11:17
    Let me mention one thing: rainbows are not  always bad if you have categorical attributes,
  • 00:11:24
    then you do want very, very bright colors
  • 00:11:27
    because the ability to perceive  small colors is dependent on size.
  • 00:11:31
    So if you have little bits of color scattered  around, we want saturated color. And of course the
  • 00:11:37
    most saturated colors we can get are those rainbow  colors. So if we have a segmented color map,
  • 00:11:43
    where we're doing something categorical rather  than a continuous one, then in fact rainbow colors
  • 00:11:49
    can be very good choices. So, it all depends on matching
  • 00:11:53
    the characteristics of your visual encoding to  the characteristics of your data set, as always.
  • 00:12:02
    So I alluded to this idea that maybe there's  some interaction between channels. We really
  • 00:12:07
    have to be careful about this with color. Color does not have separable characteristics.
  • 00:12:13
    In particular as I mentioned, size  heavily affects the salience of color.
  • 00:12:19
    So and in particular, if you have  small regions you need highly saturated
  • 00:12:25
    colors to be able to even notice them. That's why we have highly saturated red maps
  • 00:12:31
    on top of these larger background swaths of pale,  more unsaturated, colors in the map on the left.
  • 00:12:38
    Now if you have large regions you typically  would want those to be low saturation.
  • 00:12:42
    Notice how these big, big areas of highly  saturated color really jump out at you and
  • 00:12:47
    they're quite jarring so we want to be aware of  the size as we think about the saturation to use.
  • 00:12:54
    That can be tricky in a context where  you don't know ahead of time how large
  • 00:12:58
    a region will be if you've got, of course, a  data-driven situation. So there's nuance there.
  • 00:13:06
    Some key things to know about saturation  and luminance. The most crucial is, they are
  • 00:13:10
    not separable from each other. If you encode with  one, you cannot usefully encode with the other.
  • 00:13:17
    And moreover, they're not separable  from transparency either. So these
  • 00:13:21
    three channels of saturation,  and luminance, and transparency,
  • 00:13:24
    are all, you have to use only one of them.  They they very much are not separable.
  • 00:13:31
    Here's just an example with the same tool. And  this tool, by the way, is called colorbrewer. It's
  • 00:13:36
    a great tool for generating some color palettes  that's quite informed by visualization design
  • 00:13:43
    principles. So notice how what I've done on the  left here, is I've got fully saturated colors
  • 00:13:53
    that I've specified in the middle there, but
  • 00:13:57
    the transparency is reasonably high. But because I've got some transparency
  • 00:14:04
    now, these look unsaturated. And in contrast, when I have the
  • 00:14:08
    unsaturated colors on the right, and  then I have the transparency all the way,
  • 00:14:13
    notice how I can't tell the difference. So I'm unable to tell the difference
  • 00:14:17
    of transparency versus saturation. So, key  principle; definitely keep that in mind.
  • 00:14:23
    We're typically going to use transparency  often in the design of making visual layers
  • 00:14:29
    as we will talk about later, but we  don't usually try to explicitly encode
  • 00:14:35
    with transparency, because it is so hard to  separate out from luminance and saturation.
  • 00:14:44
    In general, if you're going to have small  separated regions, the safe thing to do is
  • 00:14:47
    to have only two bins. And, of course, remember to  only use one of either saturation or luminance or
  • 00:14:53
    transparency. Absolute max for things like  saturation coding for separated regions,
  • 00:14:59
    would be something like three to four bins. So for contiguous regions, you can have many
  • 00:15:06
    bins if you're going to be making distinctions  of things right next to each other. But,
  • 00:15:10
    again, be careful not to try to use  these three channels simultaneously.
  • 00:15:18
    Let's talk about color palettes. Now we've  already started down this, with thinking
  • 00:15:23
    about things like categorical colors. These are all color. We're talking
  • 00:15:29
    right now about palettes for a single  attribute univariate color palettes.
  • 00:15:33
    So we really want maximum distinguishability. That  sometimes means we go for as saturated as we can,
  • 00:15:39
    although you could also have less saturated ones.  Sometimes these are called qualitative or nominal,
  • 00:15:45
    particularly in the geographic literature  if you're using tools built after that.
  • 00:15:51
    So if you have ordered data then we really  distinguish between sequential and diverging.
  • 00:15:57
    So sequential is when you're going from min  to max, and diverging is when there's some
  • 00:16:01
    semantically meaningful midpoint. Typically  we're going to use a neutral color for that,
  • 00:16:05
    like white or yellow or gray, and then we'll pick  a saturated color for each of these end points.
  • 00:16:11
    And we see a bunch of examples of  these diverging palettes - some
  • 00:16:14
    with yellow, some with white midpoints. Now if you're doing something sequential,
  • 00:16:19
    then we've got this ramp going  from a minimum to a maximum value,
  • 00:16:23
    where typically it's unsaturated on  one side and then fully saturated or
  • 00:16:28
    fully bright versus dark, depending on  whether we're thinking about this as
  • 00:16:32
    luminance or saturation. But those are quite hard  to tell apart we want to pick just one typically.
  • 00:16:38
    So here's a few examples of where we're doing  sequential color maps, and we have multiple hues,
  • 00:16:45
    and so we are being careful to order  them by luminance as we talked about.
  • 00:16:51
    Finally there's one more property we  can think about. Remember that you can
  • 00:16:55
    have ordered data that's cyclic, and you can  emphasize the cyclic nature of that by having
  • 00:17:00
    many hues. So multi-hue maps are useful for  trying to show the cyclic nature of the data.
  • 00:17:09
    So the design considerations for a color palette  for a single attribute are, first of all,
  • 00:17:15
    are we looking at segmented discrete little  boxes, or something that's continuous?
  • 00:17:21
    Are we looking at diverging or sequential or  cyclic as the properties of the data set we want
  • 00:17:26
    to emphasize? Do we have one hue or two hues  or multiple hues? Is it perceptually linear?
  • 00:17:33
    And have we ordered the hues by luminance? And  as we'll get to later, is it colorblind safe?
  • 00:17:42
    Well, we had been talking about color for  one attribute. What if we want to encode two
  • 00:17:46
    different attributes with color? These  are what's called bivariate color maps.
  • 00:17:52
    Now there is a straightforward case where
  • 00:17:55
    we have two different attributes, but one of  those attributes is binary. It's just on or off.
  • 00:18:01
    So here's an example of that where essentially  we've got a variation in hue, and then we have the
  • 00:18:09
    low saturation and the high saturation version of  that. So what's getting encoded with saturation
  • 00:18:14
    is just binary on or off. People are  reasonably able to deal with those.
  • 00:18:21
    But in the cases where you actually have multiple  levels in each of two different attributes,
  • 00:18:28
    it gets very tricky people do use these sometimes,  but there's a fair amount of empirical evidence
  • 00:18:34
    that people find them difficult to interpret.  Not impossible, but certainly difficult.
  • 00:18:41
    So be aware that it is a much more tricky design  problem to have bivariate color maps with multiple
  • 00:18:49
    levels in each of the directions as opposed  to just binary in one of those directions.
Tags
  • Visualization
  • Color
  • Luminance
  • Saturation
  • Hue
  • Categorical Data
  • Sequential Maps
  • Diverging Maps
  • Bivariate Maps
  • Color Palettes